Submit Paper

Article Processing Fee

Pay Online

           

Crossref logo

  DOI Prefix   10.20431


 

ARC Journal of Neuroscience

Volume-1 Issue-2, 2016

Single Seizure and Cognition: Clinical and Translational Implications
Victor A. Aniol1, Alla B. Guekht2, Natalia V. Gulyaeva1,2

1.AUTHOR DETAILS
3.INTRODUCTION
4.REFERENCES

AUTHOR DETAILS

Victor A. Aniol1, Alla B. Guekht2, Natalia V. Gulyaeva1,2

1Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and Neurophysiology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Moscow, Russia
2Moscow Research and Clinical Center for Neuropsychiatry of the Healthcare Department, Moscow, Russia
[email protected], [email protected]


Abstract

Nowadays, there is no definite answer to the question whether a single seizure episode can influence cognitive functions. However, elucidating this issue has a great clinical significance, since adequate therapeutic interventions may be needed soon after the seizure occurrence if long-lasting cognitive impairments can be launched shortly after a seizure. The existing clinical observations in patients with new-onset epilepsy often imply the pre-existing brain pathology which may lead to both epileptogenesis and deterioration of mental functions. On the other hand, in some cases a positive correlation between seizure severity (frequency) and the degree of cognitive deterioration can be demonstrated suggesting that an effect of a single seizure episode may be negligible. In any case, objective difficulties of clinical investigations of seizure-induced cognitive impairments make the experimental studies on this issue highly actual, and some recent advance in this area suggests the existence of long-lasting cognitive effects of even a single seizure episode. Possible mechanisms and potential clinical significance of this phenomenon are discussed.

Keywords: seizure, single seizure episode, epilepsy, cognitive, hippocampus, neurogenesis



INTRODUCTION

Cognitive deficits are a well-known complication that often accompanies chronic epilepsy in humans [1, 2]. Neuropsychiatric problems are observed in 30-50% of patients with epilepsy [3]. Impairment of memory, primarily the short-term one, in association with epilepsy have been reported at least since the 1950-s when impairments in tactile learning and auditory perceptual tests have been documented [4]. Later, Loisseau et al. described impairments in verbal and nonverbal memory in a large group of patients with epilepsy [5]. In 1980-s a lot of data was collected, allowing to make some conclusions on the specificity of cognitive impairments depending on seizure type and localization of the focus [6]. Usually, generalized epilepsies are associated with attention deficits, while partial seizures, specifically those with a focus in the temporal lobe – with memory impairments. Moreover, according to the conventional point of view, localization of epileptic focus in the left hemisphere is associated with verbal memory impairment, whereas right-side localization is linked with nonverbal memory deterioration [7]. In addition, depending on the brain area involved in the propagation of epileptic activity, epilepsy may be accompanied by occurrence of auditory, olfactory, gustatory, somatic and visual hallucinatory phenomena [8].

Cognitive impairment is determined by factors directly associated with the epilepsy-related characteristics (age at onset, disease duration, duration and frequency of seizures, localization of epileptic focus and reasons for its formation, status epilepticus history), as well as antiepileptic therapy used [3,9-11]. Involvement of specific brain structures, severity and frequency of seizures, epileptic form EEG activity are associated with higher risk of cognitive impairment in patients with epilepsy. Aldenkamp et al. reported three specific types of cognitive disorders in epilepsy: the lack of memory, the lack of attention, and the violation of the "speed factor" - the slowness of information learning and skills acquisition [12]. Recently, other forms of cognitive impairments occurring in patients with epilepsy were described: transient epileptic amnesia, accelerated long-term forgetting, and remote memory impairment (See [13] for review).

Cognitive impairments were well documented in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), however, epilepsies originating from other brain areas may also be accompanied by cognitive dysfunctions [14]. In contrast to TLE, the epilepsy arising from the frontal lobes demonstrates no specific pattern of cognitive dysfunction. For example, patients with frontal lobe epilepsy demonstrate impaired motor coordination and response inhibition without any correlation with lesions visible on MRI [15]. As well, switching between tasks and social recognition (like humor appreciation and ability to detect emotional expression) was impaired in patients with frontal lobe epilepsy [16,17]. Sometimes, abnormalities of the long-term memory could also occur in frontal lobe epilepsy [18]. Possibly, there is no difference between TLE and frontal lobe epilepsy in associated memory impairments [19]. In contrast, occipital lobe epilepsy is characterized by decreased performance on visuospatial tasks, whereas patients with temporal epilepsy demonstrate impaired verbal long-term memory [20]. Patients with occipital lobe epilepsy often demonstrate visual hallucinations, however only in 1/3 of cases the underlying brain pathology may be visualized on MRI [21]. Patients with parietal lobe epilepsy exhibit a somatosensory aura with complex visual or auditory hallucinations and automatisms in case of temporolimbic propagation [22]. New-onset epilepsy accompanied by attention deficit is characterized by diffuse bilateral thinning of the cortex in the frontal, parietal and temporal lobes, with volume reductions in the brainstem and subcortical structures [23].

All this evidence, however, does not answer the question on the initial cause of epilepsy-related cognitive pathology. Indeed, memory or attention deficits and emotional changes may be the consequence of repeated seizure activity, as well as of the underlying brain pathology; additional complications may be induced by antiepileptic drugs, a number of them influencing the mental sphere [24]. In case if cognitive impairments develop independently of seizure occurrence and are determined by a pre-existing brain pathology, only etiotropic therapy could relieve both seizures and mental symptoms. On the other hand, if cognitive decline is caused by the epileptic activity itself, all measures should be taken for prevention of both seizure recurrence and forthcoming effect on cognition, using a specific therapy as early as possible. Thus, the main issue to be clarified is whether the seizure activity can lead to enduring cognitive impairments and which effect can be induced by just a single episode of seizures.

In this review we will focus on the existing data concerning long-lasting consequences of a single seizure. However, since such studies are quite rare, we have also included information about cognitive functions in patients with newly-diagnosed epilepsy before the start of antiepileptic treatment. We have taken into account only convulsive seizures, including partial, generalized seizures, or even status epilepticus. However, in most cases the generalized tonic clonic seizures are the most prominent and frequent feature of epilepsy syndromes. In fact, as demonstrated by CAROLE ("Coordination Active du Réseau Observatoire Longitudinal de l'Epilepsie") study, the seizures most likely present as a first seizure are generalized (including secondarily generalized) tonic-clonic seizures [24,25]. In the first chapter of our study we have tried to give an overview of existing clinical observations of an influence of convulsive seizure per se on cognitive functions (Table). One group of data comprised several studies where non-epileptic patients who have presented a single episode of convulsive seizure due to pathologies of different origin were compared with those who didn't. Another group of studies assessed the cognitive functions in untreated patients with newly-diagnosed epilepsy. Validity of these studies for our objectives is compromised because in most of them no difference was made between patients with only one and several seizures; however, effects of rare seizure activity versus effects of long-lasting chronic epilepsy still could be elucidated. Finally, some information could be obtained from studies of cognitive effects of electrically-induced seizures in neuropsychiatric patients, but these data are less relevant for our purpose because of concomitant disease factor which can mask the effect of seizure per se. Though less valid for extrapolation to human pathology, animal studies can provide more detailed information and avoid the contaminating effect of chronic disease. Therefore, in the second part of our review we present the existing observations of deterioration of cognitive functions in widely applied animal models of epilepsy and single convulsive seizure. Finally, we briefly discuss the possible mechanisms which may underlie the enduring effect of a single seizure.





* An average of 2.5 seizure episodes. MMSE - Mini-Mental State Examination; mRS - Modified Rankin Scale; NEWQOL - Quality of Life in Newly Diagnosed Epilepsy Instrument; WAIS - Wechsler adult intelligence scale; HADS - Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; ABNAS - AB Neuropsychological Assessment Schedule; AFSII - Austin First Seizure Impact Interview; RAVLT - Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; CMS - Children Memory Scale; WISC-III - Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third edition.

CLINICAL DATA

Non-Epileptic Seizures

In adults, effects of a single isolated unprovoked seizure have been evaluated in studies on focal symptomatic (or structural-metabolic) epilepsies in stroke, brain trauma, etc. De Reuck et al. compared 125 patients with late seizures after an ischemic stroke (66 patients with a single convulsion and 59 with recurrent seizures or epilepsy) with 125 patients without seizures after stroke (within 2 years) using MMSE (Mini-Mental State Examination) and mRS (modified Rankin Scale) protocols [26]. Recurrent seizures were associated with a higher mRS index as compared with patients without seizures, while the patients with only one seizure did not differ from the seizure-free group, indicating no influence of a single convulsion on the mental state of stroke patients. Similar results were obtained by Claassen et al. who analyzed 247 cases of patients with subarachnoid hemorrhage survived for one year [27]. In 7 % (17 patients) the hemorrhage was complicated by epilepsy (two or more seizures), in 4% (10 patients) only one episode of seizures occurred. The incidence of seizures was not accompanied by a decline in cognitive function for at least 12 months after the hemorrhage, although the method used for cognitive assessment (telephone interview) leaves the possibility for under-estimation of possible light impairments. However, these results were additionally supported by the data obtained in children by Strudwick et al., who compared children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes with reported seizures or coma induced by hypoglycemia with those without episodes of severe hypoglycemia [28]. A comprehensive battery of tests for learning and memory did not reveal any difference between the groups. Thus, a single convulsive episode in the context of pre-existing focal brain lesion does not seem to be associated with the development of cognitive impairments in patients. Similarly, in the rare cases of genetically determined diseases like Lafora disease and the syndrome of ring chromosome 20, the rapid development of cognitive impairment after seizure onset does not result from the first convulsion but rather is due to progression of the underlying disease inducing neuronal damage (like extensive neuronal accumulation of misfolded proteins caused by mutations in the genes encoding proteins laforin or malin in case of Lafora disease) [29,30].

A separate group of studies addresses the effects of electrically-induced seizures after electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) which is believed to be an effective approach to treat depression and some other psychiatric disorders. However, the main bulk of evidence concerning the effect of ECT seizures on cognition was obtained in patients suffering from depression, either comparing them to non-treated patients or with their own pre-treatment cognitive status. It was convincingly established (the results of numerous studies were reviewed by Calev et al.) that most prominent cognitive deterioration was observed in acute (up to several hours after the seizure) and early sub-acute period (up to several weeks after the seizure), whereas later (for several months after the ECT) no influence on cognition or even improved cognitive performance could be detected [31,32]. Strictly speaking, it is not easy to evaluate the effect of seizures on cognition in these examples since a relief of depression symptoms brought by ECT may mask effects of seizures per se.

New-Onset Epilepsy

One could expect to get evidence confirming or excluding possible effects of the first seizure on cognition from new-onset epilepsy studies. However, the information on possible cognitive impairments in these patients remains limited and clinical studies on this issue are methodologically difficult to perform [33]. In most studies, patients with new-onset epilepsy have experienced more than two seizures in a year preceding the cognitive assessment and are not suitable for the accurate assessment of the first seizure effects. It has been shown on a large group of patients that already after one-year history of epilepsy (with 4 or more seizures) patients demonstrate decreased attention (1/4), decreased memory (1/4), or both (1/4); while only half a of affected patients realized their impairments [34]. From a recent study in elderly patients with new-onset focal epilepsy before initiation of anti-epileptic treatment it was concluded that risk factors for cognitive impairment were neurological status and body mass index, rather than seizure frequency or severity and suggested that routine screening before treatment initiation is highly recommended in these patients [35].

Impaired verbal memory, visual memory, and increased reaction time were reported in patients with newly diagnosed epilepsy [36]. Hermann et al. have studied a group of 75 children (8-18 years) with newly diagnosed idiopathic epilepsy and compared it with a group of healthy children [37]. Questionnaire for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for children (K-SADS), as well as interviewing parents and school performance analysis were used to assess the preceding disorders. It has been found that attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is more common in children with epilepsy than in the group of healthy children (31% compared to 6 %). In addition, the majority of children with newly diagnosed epilepsy debuted with ADHD even before the first convulsion. Possibly it could be determined by pre-existing structural brain pathology appearing in altered volumes of the corpus callosum and whole brain white and/or gray matter tissue [38, 39]. Similar results were obtained when evaluating behavioral problems before the first seizure and four months after it in a group of children aged 4 to 15 years, who demonstrated behavioral abnormalities before the seizures in 24 % of cases [40]. The application of special educational programs in 613 children with newly diagnosed epilepsy was explored as a part of a large prospective study [24]. In 15 % of children with idiopathic and cryptogenic epilepsy, a need for special education was noted even before the first convulsion (in 7.3 % of children with the first seizure in the first 5 years of life, 20% of children with a first seizure between 5 and 9 years old, and 25% of children with a first seizure over the age of 10 years). The academic performance and the results of neurophysiological examinations were compared in children (8-18 years) with newly diagnosed idiopathic epilepsy and healthy children. Despite the children with epilepsy demonstrated the signs of mild diffuse cognitive decline and deterioration of learning achievements in comparison with the control group, a portion of children with newly diagnosed epilepsy demonstrated learning problems even before the occurrence of the first seizure [41].

Velissaris et al. examined the presence of cognitive impairment and mood disorders in 90 patients with the first convulsion at the age of 18 to 65 years one and three months after the seizure [42]. For subjective assessment of cognitive function by the patients a qualitative questionnaire Austin First Seizure Impact Interview (AFSII) and a semi-quantitative scale AB Neuropsychological Assessment Schedule (ABNAS) was used, while to assess the scale of emotional changes Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) was applied. The level of attention was examined objectively by a battery of computerized tasks. Both 1 month and 3 months after seizure, from 1/4 to 1/3 of patients complained for reduction of their cognitive functions, noting slow thinking, difficulty in remembering, forgetfulness, impaired concentration, and speech difficulties. These impairments were associated with increased depressive and anxiety indices, however, did not correlate with the level of attention. Patients with subsequent recurrent seizures showed more severe mood disorders and noted a greater cognitive decline not associated with the level of attention even before the appearance of repeated seizures. The authors suggested a leading role of emotional disorders in the development of cognitive decline in patients after first convulsions. Thus, cognitive impairments observed after the first seizures in the newly diagnosed epilepsy are closely related to emotional changes, and are not necessarily directly caused by the seizures since in some cases they precede the seizures onset. Emotional changes also often precede the clinical onset of epilepsy indicating the occurrence of independent parallel processes of motor manifestations of seizures and emotional disorders. Summarizing these observations, Rösche et al. focused on currently available data on the development of cognitive decline at an early stage of epileptogenesis both in genetically-induced and idiopathic epilepsy [33]. They suggested that increased depression and frequency of suicide attempts in adults often precede the onset of epilepsy, and in children behavioral disorders may occur before the first recognized seizure [33].

The occurrence of cognitive decline soon after the onset of epilepsy is often associated with organic brain pathology. Thus, Hermann et al. observed the dynamics of intellectual development in healthy children aged 8-18 years (n = 48) compared with matched age group of children with newly diagnosed epilepsy (n = 52) within two years after the epilepsy onset [43]. Mental development was assessed depending on the presence/absence of ADHD, educational achievements, as well as in different neuropsychological tests. The first two years of epilepsy were not accompanied by a significant decline in cognitive function in children without concomitant neurological pathology. If the epilepsy started on the background of concomitant neurological disorders, children showed significantly lower levels of cognitive development both at the beginning of the study and during the two-year observation period. Similarly, basing on the comparison of the data of Dutch epilepsy study with data received in other studies, Arts and Geerts concluded that the development of cognitive impairments in children immediately after the onset of epilepsy is rare and usually occurs only in clinical syndromes like Landau - Kleffner syndrome with continuous spikes and waves during sleep, West syndrome, Lennox - Gastaut syndrome, or Dravet syndrome, requiring an early start of an aggressive antiepileptic therapy [44]. This opinion is shared by Shinnar and Hauser who suggest that cognitive impairment after single seizures develops only in certain clinical syndromes and is associated with the development of the disease within this syndrome but not with observed convulsions per se [45]. Patients with TLE were most systematically investigated for brain pathology on MRI, and it is known that volumetric abnormaities, predominantly in form of atrophy, are evident both within and outside the primary epileptogenic region [46].It has been found that subjects with TLE demonstrate diminished volumes of total gray and white matter on MRI and increased volume of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). Moreover, the CSF volume positively correlates with the impairments assessed by neuropsychological test battery [47]. Interestingly, not only total CSF volume, but also its contents correlate with neuronal damage in patients with epilepsy; such association was found for ubiquitin carboxy-terminal hydrolase, the enzyme elevated in patients with more frequent and prolonged seizures [48].On the other hand, somatostatin level was decreased in the CSF of patients with epilepsy and did not correlate with psychological memory scores [49].

Cognitive impairments in patients with newly diagnosed epilepsy often correlate with seizure strength and frequency. Sogawa et al. presented the results of a long study of children with epilepsy and single seizure [50]. The data of neuropsychological testing, analysis of school performance and structured questionnaires showed that severity of cognitive impairment correlated positively with the number of seizures. In the group of children with single seizure, 28 % repeated year in the school, the group with 2 - 9 seizures represented 34 % of such children, and in the group with more than 10 seizures - 64 %. Indices of mental development in children with one seizure were higher than in children with epilepsy and did not differ significantly from the performance of their siblings controls. Similar results were obtained earlier by Abetz et al. in a group of 108 adult patients with newly diagnosed epilepsy, using a structured questionnaire NEWQOL: the severity of impairments of memory, reading skills, motor disturbances, poor concentration, and fatigue expression positively correlated with the frequency and severity of seizures [51].

Since the severity of seizure-related cognitive impairment in clinical studies depends on the frequency of seizures and their strength, it is difficult to reliably reveal an effect of a single seizure episode. However, longer duration of epilepsy does not necessarily mean a greater cognitive impairment. For example, verbal memory was already impaired in newly-diagnosed patients with epileptic focus in left temporal lobe even before the start of AED treatment, and these patients showed no deterioration at 5year follow-up [52].

The controversy between the studies claiming the influence of seizure duration and strength on cognitive status and those which negate such influence could be explained by different study designs, methods applied, and heterogeneity of patient cohorts. The difficulties of clinical investigations of cognitive impairments related to seizures onset make the experimental studies on this issue highly actual.

EXPERIMENTAL DATA

Impaired functions of learning and memory were reported in different animal models of acute and chronic seizures [53]. Two most widespread chemoconvulsants which are used to induce seizures in experiment, glutamate receptor agonist kainic acid and muscarinic acetylcholine receptor agonist pilocarpine, utilize a paradigm of a chronically altered epileptic brain. Indeed, both kainate and pilocarpine seizure models begin from a profound epileptic status which is later followed by spontaneous recurrent seizures. In these models, serious neuronal loss is observed in susceptible structures, namely in the CA3 area of the hippocampus, amygdala, and adjacent piriform and enthorhinal cortex, a pattern very similar to that observed in human TLE. All these similarities to human epilepsy (pattern of neuronal loss and spontaneous seizures) made kainate and pilocarpine very suitable for experimental modeling of epilepsy; however, exactly the same features of these models are undesirable for investigation of effects of a single seizure.

The seizures elicited in rodents by kainic acid and NMDA resulted in long-term dysfunctions of memory and learning, demonstrated mainly in the spatial hippocampus-dependent tasks like Morris water maze test and radial arm maze [54-58]. Emotional memory was also affected according to impaired perfromance in the passive avoidance task, a test where an animal learns to avoid an environment in which an aversive stimulus was previously delivered [59]. Cognitive deterioration was evident several days after generalized seizures and lasted for up to 5 months [55,58,60]. Similar results indicating long-lasting impairment of learning and memory were received using pilocarpineinduced seizures [61-66]. These observations were additionally reinforced by the data obtained from such chronic epilepsy/epileptogenesis models like electric kindling or pentylenetetrazole (PTZ)induced kindling (PTZK), models utilizing chronic administration of chemoconvulsant agent or repetitive electrical stimulation of epileptogenic structures like amygdala or hippocampus. Soon after the completion of PTZK rats demonstrated decreased shuttle-box performance and impaired learning in spatial tasks: radial-arm and water maze [67-76].

The most simple and widely utilized explanation of memory and learning impairments in these studies seems to be the development of neuronal damage to limbic structures, mainly the hippocampus [58]. Indeed, histological findings after kainic acid- and pilocarpine-induced seizures include profound neuronal damage in the hippocampus, amygdala, piriform and entorhinal cortex [58,77-80].Similar to the above mentioned acute seizure models, cognitive dysfunction after PTZK was accompanied by neuronal degeneration in CA1, CA3 subfields and dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus [72,81-83]. Moreover, protection against seizure-induced neuronal damage attenuated the subsequent cognitive deficits [72]. Since the severity of neuronal damage was strongly associated with number of seizures and their strength, the observations of neurodegeneration satisfactory explained the development of cognitive decline in these seizure models on one hand, and related its development to the number and severity of seizures on the other [84,85]. It should be also noted that spontaneous recurrent seizures could be observed in pilocarpine, kainate, or kindling models, thus making these models irrelevant for analysis of the long-term effects of a single seizure.

Unlike other models of acute and chronic seizures, single episodes of PTZ-induced generalized clonic-tonic convulsion were shown not to induce neuronal loss in the hippocampus, and, therefore, for a long time this model was not regarded in a context of potential subsequent cognitive impairments [84-88]. However, Erdoğan et al. have demonstrated impaired fear-related memory in rats one and two weeks after PTZ-induced seizures [89]. Later, Assaf et al. further supported these findings by the demonstration of spatial and object memory deficits in mice that experienced episodes of PTZ-induced convulsions 3-5 weeks earlier [90]. Recently, we have performed a detailed long lasting behavioral study and demonstrated slowly developing impairments of short-term memory in rats after single PTZ-evoked seizure, appearing approximately 2 months after the seizure episode [91]. Thus, there is reliable experimental evidence indicating that cognitive impairments after PTZ-induced seizures may develop independently of major tissue damage.

POTENTIAL MECHANISMS OF FIRST SEIZURE EFFECTS ON COGNITIVE FUNCTION

If not neuronal cell loss, which cellular and molecular mechanisms can be responsible for cognitive function decline induced by a single seizure episode? Along with neuronal damage, seizures induce a number of other long-lasting microstructural changes in the neuronal circuitry; one of them is the altered hippocampal neurogenesis. The data on the influence of seizure activity on neurogenesis in the mature brain are rapidly growing. Increases in hippocampal cell proliferation have been reported after kainate- or pilocarpine-induced seizures [92-95]. Jiang et al. showed that increased proliferation was maintained for up to 14 days after seizures, gradually declining to the control level [96]. Parent et al. have demonstrated that increased neurogenesis after seizures might contribute to aberrant reorganization of hippocampal network; later this point of view was supported and further developed by other authors [97-99]. The time period from cell birth to incorporation of newly generated cells into the functional hippocampal circuits in the temporal profile of adult neurogenesis roughly corresponds to a delayed appearance of memory dysfunction in rodents [100,101]. Thus, altered hippocampal neurogenesis is one of the first candidate mechanisms for seizure induced slowly developing long-lasting cognitive impairments. This hypothesis was supported by the observations demonstrating that suppression of adult neurogenesis may improve cognitive function in seizureexperienced brain [57,102].

An important finding is that newly born neurons appearing shortly after status epilepticus and surviving during the time necessary for their functional integration into the neuronal circuit might demonstrate decreased excitability even in the absence of major morphological abnormalities [103]. In this case cognitive impairments could be explained by deteriorated passage of stimuli through the perforant pathway to the newly born dentate gyrus granule cells, which are currently regarded as key elements responsible for adaptation to novelty [104]. The main feature of animals experienced seizures may be not just the elevated number of neurons born after seizures but rather their qualitative abnormality: altered electrophysiological properties of survived newborn neurons. This concept corresponds with the data indicating that cognitive decline after seizures is accompanied by normal values of survived adult-born granule cells in the dentate gyrus of rats after a seizure episode [91]. It has been shown that neurons born short after seizures exhibit atypical spiny basal dendrites extending into the hilus of the dentate gyrus [57]. In addition, an ectopic hilar migration of granule cells generated after seizures is accompanied by atypical synchronization of their electric activity with CA3 pyramidal neurons [105]. Furthermore, extensive mossy fiber sprouting is characteristic for granule cells appeared after seizures [106]. Taken together, these alterations illustrate that long-lasting consequence of a single seizure is likely an abnormal integration of newly-generated neurons but not just an increased number of new neurons. Later, altered local circuits in turn may provoke both further seizure progression and memory impairments.

The neuroinflammation should be considered in the first place among potential causes for altered maturation of new-born neurons. This important process including an activation of microglial cells at the cellular level and elevated release of interleukins as the main molecular trigger takes place in the brain after seizures [107-109]. Indeed, interaction between inflammatory reactions in the brain and seizure development is a well-known phenomenon (see [110] for review). In some cases, epilepsy progression seems to be determined by an autoimmune response following a single breakdown of the blood-brain barrier [111]. On the other hand, generalized convulsions including those provoked by PTZ induce transient blood-brain barrier opening thus providing a risk for activation of neuroinflammatory processes in the brain [112].

The fate of newly generated cells in a long-term perspective may be also influenced by altered by a seizure episode local concentrations of neurotransmitters and neuromodulators like aspartate, glutamate, and GABA [76,113]. A single PTZ-induced convulsion induces increased expression of neuronal NO-synthase, NO production, or NO metabolites levels [114-116]. This may be particularly important since NO is a well-known regulator of proliferation and neurogenesis in the hippocampus [117-119] (see also [120] for review). Single neonatal seizure induces modifications in the distribution of superficial ionic transporters on the cell membrane of hippocampal neurons [121]. This may result in the changes in neuronal response to neurotransmitters and, as a result, in impaired hippocampal functioning.

Other possible mechanisms of seizure-induced decrease of memory and learning abilities may include impaired function of synaptic machinery proteins, slowly developing neuronal damage that could not be found at shorter time spans after the seizures, or establishment of aberrant synaptic connections in the dentate gyrus [122-124].


CONCLUSION

The existing clinical data related to potential effects of a single seizure "per se" are rare and do not allow to make a definite conclusion due to differences in the design of the studies, various pathologies accompanied by seizures (from symptomatic seizures to the first seizures in the context of epileptogenesis), different severity of underlying organic brain pathology. However, a brief review of clinical studies in this field does not exclude the possibility of negative impact of isolated seizure episode on the mental state of patients. On the other hand, limitations typical for clinical studies can be simply avoided using specifically designed animal models, and the results obtained in these models suggest that a single seizure episode can induce a slowly developing process of cognitive decline. The retarded development of cognitive decline in rodents (the changes can be reliably demonstrated after several months after the seizure episode) may explain additional important difficulties of respective clinical studies: seizure induced supposed cognitive decline in humans may be evident after many months. In addition, animal models are useful for investigations of molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying seizure-induced cognitive deficits.

In conclusion, it is noteworthy that long time period between the seizure and the manifestation of cognitive decline provides a time window for possible therapeutic interventions. Approaches to such therapy should be based on data of a thorough research on animal models. Elaboration of such "postconditioning" treatments would provide a promising opportunity to prevent potential mental impairments in patients who have experienced seizure episode(s).

Supported by RFBR grant #16-04-01513 (VA)

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Supported by RFBR grant #16-04-01513 (VA)

REFERENCES

  1. Brooks-Kayal A., Molecular mechanisms of cognitive and behavioral comorbidities of epilepsy in children, Epilepsia. 52(Suppl 1), 13-20 (2011).
  2. Carreño M., Donaire A., and Sánchez-Carpintero R., Cognitive disorders associated with epilepsy: diagnosis and treatment, Neurologist. 14(6 Suppl 1), S26-34 (2008).
  3. Trimble M. and Schmitz B., Seizures, affective disorders and anticonvulsant drugs. Guildford, U.K.: Clarus Press Ltd, 2002.
  4. Deutsch C.P., Differences among epileptics and between epileptics and nonepileptics in terms of some memory and learning variables, AMA Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry. 70(4), 474-82 (1953).
  5. Loisseau P., Stube E., Broustet D., Battelleochi S., Gomeni C., and Morsell P.D., Evaluation of memory function in a population of epileptic patients and matched controls, Acta Neurol. Scand. Suppl. 80, 58-61 (1980).
  6. Trimble M.R. and Thompson P.J., Neuropsychological and behavioral sequelae of spontaneous seizures, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 462, 284-92 (1986).
  7. Ladavas E., Umilta C., and Provinciali L., Hemisphere-dependent cognitive performances in epileptic patients, Epilepsia. 20(5), 493-502 (1979).
  8. Elliott B., Joyce E., and Shorvon S., Delusions, illusions and hallucinations in epilepsy: 1. Elementary phenomena, Epilepsy Res. 85(2-3), 162-71 (2009).
  9. Pohlmann-Eden B., Aldenkamp A., Baker G.A., Brandt C., Cendes F., Coras R., Crocker C.E., Helmstaedter C., Jones-Gotman M., Kanner A.M., Mazarati A., Mula M., Smith M.L., Omisade A., Tellez-Zenteno J., and Hermann B.P., The relevance of neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive problems in new-onset epilepsy - Current knowledge and understanding, Epilepsy Behav. 51,199-209 (2015).
  10. Avanzini G., Depaulis A., Tassinari A., and de Curtis M., Do seizures and epileptic activity worsen epilepsy and deteriorate cognitive function? Epilepsia. 54 (Suppl 8),14-21 (2013).
  11. Bell B., Lin J.J., Seidenberg M., and Hermann B., The neurobiology of cognitive disorders in temporal lobe epilepsy, Nat. Rev. Neurol. 7(3),154-64 (2011).
  12. Aldenkamp A.P., An open, nonrandomized clinical comparative study evaluating the effect of epilepsy on learning, J. Child Neurol. 14(12), 795-800 (1999).
  13. Butler C.R. and Zeman A.Z., Recent insights into the impairment of memory in epilepsy: transient epileptic amnesia, accelerated long-term forgetting and remote memory impairment, Brain. 131, 2243-2263 (2008).
  14. O'Muircheartaigh J. and Richardson M.P., Epilepsy and the frontal lobes, Cortex. 48(2), 144-55 (2012).
  15. Helmstaedter C., Kemper B., and Elger C.E., Neuropsychological aspects of frontal lobe epilepsy, Neuropsychologia. 34(5), 399-406 (1996).
  16. McDonald C.R., Delis D.C., Norman M.A., Tecoma E.S., and Iragui-Madozi V.I., Is impairment in set-shifting specific to frontal-lobe dysfunction? Evidence from patients with frontallobe or temporal-lobe epilepsy, J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11(4), 477-481 (2005).
  17. Farrant A., Morris R.G., Russell T., Elwes R., Akanuma N., Alarcon G., and Koutroumanidis M., Social cognition in frontal lobe epilepsy, Epilepsy Behav. 7(3), 506-516 (2005).
  18. Centeno M., Thompson P.J., Koepp M.J., Helmstaedter C., and Duncan J.S., Memory in frontal lobe epilepsy, Epilepsy Res. 91(2-3), 123-132 (2010).
  19. Cahn-Weiner D.A., Wittenberg D., and McDonald C., Everyday cognition in temporal lobe and frontal lobe epilepsy, Epileptic Disord. 11(3), 222-7 (2009).
  20. Santangelo G., Trojano L., Vitale C., Improta I., Alineri I., Meo R., and Bilo L., Cognitive dysfunctions in occipital lobe epilepsy compared to temporal lobe epilepsy, J. Neuropsychol. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12085 (2015). [Epub ahead of print]
  21. Kotov A.S., Rudakova I.G., Belova Iu.A., and Kotov S.V., Occipital lobe epilepsy in adults, Zh. Nevrol. Psikhiatr. Im. S.S. Korsakova. 109(7), 4-8 (2009).
  22. Salanova V., Parietal lobe epilepsy, J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 29(5), 392-6 (2012).
  23. Saute R., Dabbs K., Jones J.E., Jackson D.C., Seidenberg M., and Hermann B.P., Brain morphology in children with epilepsy and ADHD, PLoS One. 9(4), e95269 (2014).
  24. Berg A.T., Smith S.N., Frobish D., Levy S.R., Testa F.M., Beckerman B., and Shinnar S., Special education needs of children with newly diagnosed epilepsy, Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 47(11), 749-53 (2005).
  25. CAROLE, D´elais evolutifs des syndromes epileptiques avant leur diagnostic: resultats descriptifs de l'enquete CAROLE, Rev. Neurol. (Paris). 156, 481–490 (2000).
  26. De Reuck J., De Clerck M., and Van Maele G., Vascular cognitive impairment in patients with late-onset seizures after an ischemic stroke, Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 108(7), 632-7 (2006).
  27. Claassen J., Peery S., Kreiter K.T., Hirsch L.J., Du E.Y., Connolly E.S., and Mayer S.A., Predictors and clinical impact of epilepsy after subarachnoid hemorrhage, Neurology. 60(2), 208-14 (2003).
  28. Strudwick S.K., Carne C., Gardiner J., Foster J.K., Davis E.A., and Jones T.W., Cognitive functioning in children with early onset type 1 diabetes and severe hypoglycemia, J. Pediatr. 147(5), 680-5 (2005).
  29. Genton P., Borg M., Vigliano P., Pellissier J.F., and Roger J., Semi-late onset and rapidly progressive case of Lafora's disease with predominant cognitive symptoms, Eur. Neurol. 29(6), 333-8 (1989).
  30. Jacobs J., Bernard G., Andermann E., Dubeau F., and Andermann F., Refractory and lethal status epilepticus in a patient with ring chromosome 20 syndrome, Epileptic Disord. 10(4), 2549 (2008).
  31. Calev A., Gaudino E.A., Squires N.K., Zervas I.M., and Fink M., ECT and non-memory cognition: a review, Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 34(Pt 4), 505-15 (1995).
  32. Semkovska M. and McLoughlin D.M., Objective cognitive performance associated with electroconvulsive therapy for depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis, Biol. Psychiatry. 68(6), 568-77 (2010).
  33. Rösche J., Uhlmann C., and Fröscher W., Cognitive deficits and psychiatric disorders in patients with new-onset epilepsy, Fortschr. Neurol. Psychiatr. 78(1), 18-26 (2010).
  34. Witt J.A. and Helmstaedter C., Should cognition be screened in new-onset epilepsies? A study in 247 untreated patients, J. Neurol. (2012) 259(8), 1727-31.
  35. Witt J.A., Werhahn K.J., Krämer G., Ruckes C., Trinka E., and Helmstaedter C., Cognitivebehavioral screening in elderly patients with new-onset epilepsy before treatment, Acta Neurol. Scand. 130(3), 172-7 (2014).
  36. Taylor J. and Baker G.A., Newly diagnosed epilepsy: cognitive outcome at 5 years, Epilepsy Behav. 18(4), 397-403 (2010).
  37. Hermann B., Jones J., Dabbs K., Allen C.A., Sheth R., Fine J., McMillan A., and Seidenberg M., The frequency, complications and aetiology of ADHD in new onset paediatric epilepsy, Brain. 130(Pt 12), 3135-48 (2007).
  38. Hermann B.P., Dabbs K., Becker T., Jones J.E., Myers y Gutierrez A., Wendt G., Koehn M.A., Sheth R., and Seidenberg M., Brain development in children with new onset epilepsy: A prospective controlled cohort investigation, Epilepsia. 51(10), 2038-2046 (2010).
  39. Hermann B.P., Jones J.E., Jackson D.C., and Seidenberg M., Starting at the beginning: The neuropsychological status of children with new onset epilepsies, Epileptic Disord. 14(1), 12-21 (2012).
  40. Dunn D.W., Austin J.K., and Huster G.A., Behaviour problems in children with new-onset epilepsy, Seizure. 6(4), 283-7 (1997).
  41. Hermann B., Jones J., Sheth R., Dow C., Koehn M., and Seidenberg M., Children with newonset epilepsy: neuropsychological status and brain structure, Brain. 129(Pt 10), 2609-19 (2006).
  42. Velissaris S.L., Wilson S.J., Newton M.R., Berkovic S.F., and Saling M.M., Cognitive complaints after a first seizure in adulthood: Influence of psychological adjustment, Epilepsia. 50(5), 1012-21 (2009).
  43. Hermann B.P., Jones J.E., Sheth R., Koehn M., Becker T., Fine J., Allen C.A., and Seidenberg M., Growing up with epilepsy: a two-year investigation of cognitive development in children with new onset epilepsy, Epilepsia. 49(11), 1847-58 (2008).
  44. Arts W.F. and Geerts A.T., When to start drug treatment for childhood epilepsy: the clinicalepidemiological evidence, Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 13(2), 93-101 (2009).
  45. Shinnar S. and Hauser W.A., Do occasional brief seizures cause detectable clinical consequences? Prog. Brain. Res. 135, 221-235 (2002).
  46. Tasch E., Cendes F., Li L.M., Dubeau F., Andermann F., and Arnold D.L., Neuroimaging evidence of progressive neuronal loss and dysfunction in temporal lobe epilepsy, Ann. Neurol. 45, 568-576 (1999).
  47. Oyegbile T.O., Bhattacharya A., Seidenberg M., and Hermann B.P., Quantitative MRI biomarkers of cognitive morbidity in temporal lobe epilepsy, Epilepsia. 47(1), 143-52 (2006).
  48. Li Y., Wang Z., Zhang B., Zhe X., Wang M., Bai J., Lin T., and Zhang S., Cerebrospinal fluid ubiquitin C-terminal hydrolase as a novel marker of neuronal damage after epileptic seizure, Epilepsy Res. 103(2-3), 205-10 (2013).
  49. Pitkänen A., Jolkkonen J., Aikiä M., Sirviö J., Sivenius J., and Riekkinen P.J., Somatostatinlike immunoreactivity in cerebrospinal fluid of patients with complex partial epilepsy, Eur. Neurol. 28(1), 1-5 (1988).
  50. Sogawa Y., Masur D., O'Dell C., Moshe S.L., and Shinnar S., Cognitive outcomes in children who present with a first unprovoked seizure, Epilepsia. 51(12), 2432-2439 (2010).
  51. Abetz L., Jacoby A., Baker G.A., and McNulty P., Patient-based assessments of quality of life in newly diagnosed epilepsy patients: validation of the NEWQOL, Epilepsia. 41(9), 1119-28 (2000).
  52. Aikiä M., Salmenperä T., Partanen K., and Kälviäinen R., Verbal Memory in Newly Diagnosed Patients and Patients with Chronic Left Temporal Lobe Epilepsy, Epilepsy Behav. 2(1), 20-27 (2001).
  53. Majak K. and Pitkänen A., Do seizures cause irreversible cognitive damage? Evidence from animal studies, Epilepsy Behav. 5(Suppl 1), S35-44 (2004).
  54. Gröticke I., Hoffmann K., and Löscher W., Behavioral alterations in a mouse model of temporal lobe epilepsy induced by intrahippocampal injection of kainate, Exp. Neurol. 213(1), 71-83 (2008).
  55. Kunz T., Marklund N., Hillered L., and Oliw E.H., Assessment of the effects of the cyclooxygenase-2 inhibitor rofecoxib on visuospatial learning and hippocampal cell death following kainate-induced seizures in the rat, Brain. Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 25(3), 826-832 (2005).
  56. Yin S., Guan Z., Tang Y., Zhao J., Hong J., and Zhang W., Abnormal expression of epilepsyrelated gene ERG1/NSF in the spontaneous recurrent seizure rats with spatial learning memory deficits induced by kainic acid, Brain Res. 1053(1-2), 195-202 (2005).
  57. Jessberger S., Nakashima K., Clemenson G.D. Jr., Mejia E., Mathews E., Ure K., Ogawa S., Sinton C.M., Gage F.H., and Hsieh J., Epigenetic modulation of seizure-induced neurogenesis and cognitive decline, J. Neurosci. 27(22), 5967-5975 (2007).
  58. Letty S., Lerner-Natoli M., and Rondouin G., Differential impairments of spatial memory and social behavior in two models of limbic epilepsy, Epilepsia. 36(10), 973-982 (1995).
  59. Kim D.H., Yoon B.H., Jung W.Y., Kim J.M., Park S.J., Park D.H., Huh Y., Park C., Cheong J.H., Lee K.T., Shin C.Y., and Ryu J.H., Sinapic acid attenuates kainic acid-induced hippocampal neuronal damage in mice, Neuropharmacology. 59(1-2), 20-30 (2010).
  60. Mikati M.A., Tarif S., Lteif L., and Jawad M.A., Time sequence and types of memory deficits after experimental status epilepticus, Epilepsy Res. 43(2), 97-101 (2001).
  61. Hort J., Brozek G., Komárek V., Langmeier M., and Mares P., Interstrain differences in cognitive functions in rats in relation to status epilepticus, Behav. Brain Res. 112(1-2), 77-83 (2000).
  62. Mohajeri M.H., Saini K., Li H., Crameri A., Lipp H.P., Wolfer D.P., and Nitsch R.M., Intact spatial memory in mice with seizure-induced partial loss of hippocampal pyramidal neurons, Neurobiol. Dis. 12(3), 174-181 (2003).
  63. Persinger M.A., Bureau Y.R., Kostakos M., Peredery O., and Falter H., Behaviors of rats with insidious, multifocal brain damage induced by seizures following single peripheral injections of lithium and pilocarpine, Physiol. Behav. 53(5), 849-866 (1993).
  64. Persinger M.A., Bureau Y.R., and Peredery O., Dissociation between conditioned taste aversion and radial maze learning following seizure-induced multifocal brain damage: quantitative tests of serial vs parallel circuit models of memory, Physiol. Behav. 56(2), 225-235 (1994).
  65. Sroubek J., Hort J., Komárek V., Langmeier M., and Brozek G., Acquisition and retrieval of conditioned taste aversion is impaired by brain damage caused by two hours of pilocarpineinduced status epilepticus, Physiol. Res. 50(6), 609-617 (2001).
  66. Yang Y., Liu Z., Cermak J.M., Tandon P., Sarkisian M.R., Stafstrom C.E., Neill J.C., Blusztajn J.K., and Holmes G.L., Protective effects of prenatal choline supplementation on seizureinduced memory impairment, J. Neurosci. 20(22), RC109 (2000).
  67. Becker A., Tiedge A., and Grecksch G.A., Diazepam – its effects on the development of pentylenetetrazol kindling, related learning impairments, and neuronal cell loss, Pharmacol. Res. 35(1), 27-32 (1997).
  68. Becker A., Braun H., Schröder H., Grecksch G., and Höllt V., Effects of enadoline on the development of pentylenetetrazol kindling, learning performance, and hippocampal morphology, Brain Res. 823(1-2), 191-197 (1999).
  69. Genkova-Papazova M.G. and Lazarova-Bakarova M.B., Pentylenetetrazole kindling impairs long-term memory in rats, Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 5(1), 53-56 (1995).
  70. Grecksch G., Becker A., Schroeder H., and Höllt V., Involvement of delta-opioid receptors in pentylenetetrazol kindling development and kindling-related processes in rats, Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol. 360(2), 151-156 (1999).
  71. Hassan H., Grecksch G., Rüthrich H., and Krug M., Effects of nicardipine, an antagonist of Ltype voltage-dependent calcium channels, on kindling development, kindling-induced learning deficits and hippocampal potentiation phenomena, Neuropharmacology. 38(12), 1841-1850 (1999).
  72. Pohle W., Becker A., Grecksch G., Juhre A., and Willenberg A., Piracetam prevents pentylenetetrazol kindling-induced neuronal loss and learning deficits, Seizure. 6(6), 467-474 (1997).
  73. Rüthrich H., Grecksch G., and Krug M., Development of long-lasting potentiation effects in the dentate gyrus during pentylenetetrazol kindling, Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 19(3), 247-254 (2001).
  74. Mortazavi F., Ericson M., Story D., Hulce V.D., and Dunbar G.L., Spatial learning deficits and emotional impairments in pentylenetetrazole-kindled rats, Epilepsy Behav. 7(4), 629-638 (2005).
  75. Omrani A., Ghadami M.R., Fathi N., Tahmasian M., Fathollahi Y., and Touhidi A., Naloxone improves impairment of spatial performance induced by pentylenetetrazol kindling in rats, Neuroscience. 145(3), 824-831 (2007).
  76. Szyndler J., Piechal A., Blecharz-Klin K., Skórzewska A., Maciejak P., Walkowiak J., Turzyńska D., Bidziński A., Płaźnik A., and Widy-Tyszkiewicz E., Effect of kindled seizures on rat behavior in water Morris maze test and amino acid concentrations in brain structures, Pharmacol. Rep. 58(1), 75-82 (2006).
  77. Haas K.Z., Sperber E.F., Opanashuk L.A., Stanton P.K., and Moshé S.L., Resistance of immature hippocampus to morphologic and physiologic alterations following status epilepticus or kindling, Hippocampus. 11(6), 615-625 (2001).
  78. Hattiangady B., Kuruba R., and Shetty A.K., Acute seizures in old age leads to a greater loss of CA1 pyramidal neurons, an increased propensity for developing chronic TLE and a severe cognitive dysfunction, Aging Dis. 2(1), 1-17 (2011).
  79. Miltiadous P., Stamatakis A., Koutsoudaki P.N., Tiniakos D.G., and Stylianopoulou F., IGF-I ameliorates hippocampal neurodegeneration and protects against cognitive deficits in an animal model of temporal lobe epilepsy, Exp. Neurol. 231(2), 223-235 (2011).
  80. Obeid M., Frank J., Medina M., Finckbone V., Bliss R., Bista B., Majmudar S., Hurst D., Strahlendorf H., and Strahlendorf J., Neuroprotective effects of leptin following kainic acidinduced status epilepticus, Epilepsy Behav. 19(3), 278-283 (2010).
  81. Franke H. and Kittner H., Morphological alterations of neurons and astrocytes and changes in emotional behavior in pentylenetetrazol-kindled rats, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 70(2-3), 291-303 (2001).
  82. Pavlova T.V., Yakovlev A.A., Stepanichev M.Y., and Gulyaeva N.V., Pentylenetetrazol kindling in rats: Is neurodegeneration associated with manifestations of convulsive activity? Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 36(7), 741-748 (2006).
  83. Pavlova T.V., Stepanichev M.Y., Gekht A.B., and Gulyaeva N.V., Acquisition of an active avoidance reaction in rats and morphological changes in the hippocampus in pentylenetetrazol kindling, Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 40(5), 501-506 (2010).
  84. Planas A.M., Soriano M.A., Ferrer I., and Rodríguez Farré E., Regional expression of inducible heat shock protein-70 mRNA in the rat brain following administration of convulsant drugs, Brain Res. Mol. Brain. Res. 27(1), 127-137 (1994).
  85. Kasof G.M., Mandelzys A., Maika S.D., Hammer R.E., Curran T., and Morgan J.I., Kainic acid-induced neuronal death is associated with DNA damage and a unique immediate-early gene response in c-fos-lacZ transgenic rats, J. Neurosci. 15(6), 4238-4249 (1995).
  86. Kadar T., Cohen G., Sahar R., Alkalai D., and Shapira S., Long-term study of brain lesions following soman, in comparison to DFP and metrazol poisoning, Hum. Exp. Toxicol. 11(6), 517-523 (1992).
  87. Panegyres P.K., The effects of excitotoxicity on the expression of the amyloid precursor protein gene in the brain and its modulation by neuroprotective agents, J. Neural Transm. 105(4-5), 463-478 (1998).
  88. Pereno G. and Beltramino C., Understanding the pathophysiology of epilepsy in an animal model: pentylenetetrazole induces activation but not death of neurons of the medial extended amygdale, Neurologia. 25(3), 148-155 (2010).
  89. Erdoğan F., Gölgeli A., Arman F., and Ersoy A.O., The effects of pentylenetetrazole-induced status epilepticus on behavior, emotional memory, and learning in rats, Epilepsy Behav. 5(3), 388-393 (2004).
  90. Assaf F., Fishbein M., Gafni M., Keren O., and Sarne Y., Pre- and post-conditioning treatment with an ultra-low dose of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) protects against pentylenetetrazole (PTZ)-induced cognitive damage, Behav. Brain Res. 220(1), 194-201 (2011).
  91. Aniol V.A., Ivanova-Dyatlova A.Y., Keren O., Guekht A.B., Sarne Y., and Gulyaeva N.V., A single pentylenetetrazole-induced clonic-tonic seizure episode is accompanied by a slowly developing cognitive decline in rats, Epilepsy Behav. 26(2), 196-202 (2013).
  92. Gray W.P. and Sundstrom L.E., Kainic acid increases the proliferation of granule cell progenitors in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat, Brain Res. 790(1-2), 52-9 (1998).
  93. Hüttmann K., Sadgrove M., Wallraff A., Hinterkeuser S., Kirchhoff F., Steinhäuser C., and Gray W.P., Seizures preferentially stimulate proliferation of radial glia-like astrocytes in the adult dentate gyrus: functional and immunocytochemical analysis, Eur. J. Neurosci. 18(10), 2769-78 (2003).
  94. Radley J.J. and Jacobs B.L., Pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus increases cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus of adult rats via a 5-HT1A receptor-dependent mechanism, Brain Res. 966(1), 1-12 (2003).
  95. Sankar R., Shin D., Liu H., Katsumori H., and Wasterlain C.G., Granule cell neurogenesis after status epilepticus in the immature rat brain, Epilepsia. 41(Suppl 6), S53-6 (2000).
  96. Jiang W., Wan Q., Zhang Z.J., Wang W.D., Huang Y.G., Rao Z.R. and Zhang X., Dentate granule cell neurogenesis after seizures induced by pentylenetrazol in rats, Brain Res. 977(2), 141-148 (2003).
  97. Parent J.M., Yu T.W., Leibowitz R.T., Geschwind D.H., Sloviter R.S., and Lowenstein D.H., Dentate granule cell neurogenesis is increased by seizures and contributes to aberrant network reorganization in the adult rat hippocampus, J. Neurosci. 17(10), 3727-3738 (1997).
  98. Nakagawa E., Aimi Y., Yasuhara O., Tooyama I., Shimada M., McGeer P.L., and Kimura H., Enhancement of progenitor cell division in the dentate gyrus triggered by initial limbic seizures in rat models of epilepsy, Epilepsia. 41(1), 10-18 (2000).
  99. Scharfman H.E., Functional implications of seizure-induced neurogenesis, Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 548, 192-212 (2004).
  100. Encinas J.M., Vaahtokari A., and Enikolopov G., Fluoxetine targets early progenitor cells in the adult brain, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 103(21), 8233-8238 (2006).
  101. Von Bohlen und Halbach O., Immunohistological markers for staging neurogenesis in adult hippocampus, Cell Tissue Res. 329(3), 409-420 (2007).
  102. Jessberger S., Clark R.E., Broadbent N.J., Clemenson G.D. Jr., Consiglio A., Lie D.C., Squire L.R., and Gage F.H., Dentate gyrus-specific knockdown of adult neurogenesis impairs spatial and object recognition memory in adult rats, Learn. Mem. 16(2), 147-154 (2009).
  103. Wood J.C., Jackson J.S., Jakubs K., Chapman K.Z., Ekdahl C.T., Kokaia Z., Kokaia M., and Lindvall O., Functional integration of new hippocampal neurons following insults to the adult brain is determined by characteristics of pathological environment, Exp. Neurol. 229(2), 484-93 (2011).
  104. Kempermann G., Youth culture in the adult brain, Science. 335(6073), 1175-6 (2012).
  105. Scharfman H.E., Goodman J.H., and Sollas A.L., Granule-like neurons at the hilar⁄ CA3 border after status epilepticus and their synchrony with area CA3 pyramidal cells: functional implications of seizure-induced neurogenesis, J. Neurosci. 20, 6144-6158 (2000).
  106. Kron M.M., Zhang H., and Parent J.M., The developmental stage of dentate granule cells dictates their contribution to seizure-induced plasticity, J. Neurosci. 30, 2051-2059 (2010).
  107. Ekdahl C.T., Kokaia Z., and Lindvall O., Brain inflammation and adult neurogenesis: the dual role of microglia, Neuroscience. 158(3), 1021-9 (2009).
  108. Zakubs K., Nanobashvili A., Bonde S., Ekdahl C.T., Kokaia Z., Kokaia M., and Lindvall O., Environment matters: synaptic properties of neurons born in the epileptic adult brain develop to reduce excitability, Neuron. 52(6), 1047-59 (2006).
  109. Jakubs K., Bonde S., Iosif R.E., Ekdahl C.T., Kokaia Z., Kokaia M., and Lindvall O., Inflammation regulates functional integration of neurons born in adult brain, J. Neurosci. 28(47), 12477-88 (2008).
  110. Vezzani A. and Granata T., Brain inflammation in epilepsy: experimental and clinical evidence, Epilepsia. 46(11), 1724-43 (2005).
  111. Choi J. and Koh S., Role of brain inflammation in epileptogenesis, Yonsei Med. J. 49(1), 1-18 (2008)
  112. Sahin D., Ilbay G., and Ates N., Changes in the blood-brain barrier permeability and in the brain tissue trace element concentrations after single and repeated pentylenetetrazole-induced seizures in rats, Pharmacol. Res. 48(1), 69-73 (2003).
  113. Rowley H.L., Marsden C.A., and Martin K.F., Generalised seizure-induced changes in rat hippocampal glutamate but not GABA release are potentiated by repeated seizures, Neurosci Lett. 234(2-3), 143-6 (1997).
  114. Bashkatova V., Vitskova G., Narkevich V., Vanin A., Mikoyan V., and Rayevsky K., Nitric oxide content measured by ESR-spectroscopy in the rat brain is increased during pentylenetetrazole-induced seizures, J. Mol. Neurosci. 14(3), 183-90 (2000).
  115. Kaneko K., Itoh K., Berliner L.J., Miyasaka K., and Fujii H., Consequences of nitric oxide generation in epileptic-seizure rodent models as studied by in vivo EPR, Magn. Reson. Med. 48(6), 1051-6 (2002).
  116. Han D., Yamada K., Senzaki K., Xiong H., Nawa H., and Nabeshima T., Involvement of nitric oxide in pentylenetetrazole-induced kindling in rats, J. Neurochem. 74(2), 792-8 (2000).
  117. Fritzen S., Schmitt A., Köth K., Sommer C., Lesch KP., and Reif A., Neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS-I) knockout increases the survival rate of neural cells in the hippocampus independently of BDNF, Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 35(2), 261-71 (2007).
  118. Park C., Sohn Y., Shin KS., Kim J., Ahn H., and Huh Y., The chronic inhibition of nitric oxide synthase enhances cell proliferation in the adult rat hippocampus, Neurosci. Lett. 339(1), 9-12 (2003).
  119. Zhou Q.G., Hu Y., Hua Y., Hu M., Luo C.X., Han X., Zhu X.J., Wang B., Xu J.S., and Zhu D.Y., Neuronal nitric oxide synthase contributes to chronic stress-induced depression by suppressing hippocampal neurogenesis, J. Neurochem. 103(5), 1843-54 (2007).
  120. Aniol V.A. and Stepanichev M.Yu., Nitric oxide and gamma-aminobutyric acid as regulators of neurogenesis in the brain of adult mammals: Models of seizure activity, Neurochemical Journal. 1(4), 265-274 (2007).
  121. Khirug S., Ahmad F., Puskarjov M., Afzalov R., Kaila K., and Blaesse P., A single seizure episode leads to rapid functional activation of KCC2 in the neonatal rat hippocampus, J. Neurosci. 30(36), 12028-12035 (2010).
  122. Dubé C., da Silva Fernandes M.J., and Nehlig A., Age-dependent consequences of seizures and the development of temporal lobe epilepsy in the rat, Dev. Neurosci. 23(3), 219-223 (2001).
  123. Scorza F.A., Arida R.M., Naffah-Mazzacoratti Mda G., Scerni D.A., Calderazzo L., and Cavalheiro E.A., The pilocarpine model of epilepsy: what have we learned? An. Acad. Bras. Cienc. 81(3), 345-365 (2009).
  124. Scharfman H.E., Epilepsy as an example of neural plasticity, Neuroscientist. 8(2), 154-173 (2002).